More on Pinochet

[ Note to regular phonoloblog readers: this is a follow-up on my last Language Log post, on the Chilean Spanish pronunciation of Pinochet. ]

Here is a key for redirected Language Log readers who may not be familiar with phonetic terms and transcriptions. The phonetic value of any other symbols used below should be transparent. (IPA = International Phonetic Alphabet, APA = [North] American[ist] Phonetic Alphabet):

  • The sound that is usually represented orthographically by “ch” in English (as in e.g. chain) is a voiceless postalveolar affricate and is represented as either [ʧ] (IPA) or [č] (APA).
  • The sound that is usually represented orthographically by “sh” in English (as in e.g. shame) is a voiceless postalveolar fricative and is represented as either [ʃ] (IPA) or [š] (APA).
  • The sound that has no single usual orthographic representation in English but is sometimes represented as “zh” (as in e.g. measure) is a voiced postalveolar fricative and is represented as either [ʒ] (IPA) or [ž] (APA).

To double-check my claims about different American Spanish varieties, I first consulted the superb book by D. Lincoln Canfield pictured and Amazon.com-linked on the right. According to the book description, “[t]his book represents the culmination of a lifetime of research in the spoken Spanish dialects of the Americas by one of the foremost experts in this field.” Indeed. Canfield makes the useful organizational decision to devote a separate chapter to the discussion of pronunciation patterns found in each country on the continent (including the U.S.), though he is clear about the fact that differences between varieties of a language do not necessarily respect national boundaries (pp. 20-21). Each chapter includes a map (in some cases, multiple maps) highlighting certain key (geographically-definable) pronunciation patterns. It’s an amazing piece of work, mercifully short (130pp.), and at $14 from Amazon.com, a real steal. (Makes a great gift, too!)

But: this book was published a full generation ago (1981), so it’s getting a little out of date. To supplement this, then, I also consulted the two books pictured and Amazon.com-linked below the fold.

The first is the book by José Ignacio Hualde that I mentioned in my Language Log post (this was the only book I happened to have on hand at the time). This book was published just last year so it’s much more recent, but it is also more of a textbook about Spanish sounds (phonetics and phonology) generally and less well-suited as an easy reference for finding out where things are pronounced how. Still, there’s lots of information about different dialects throughout the book, and it comes with an audio CD on which “[a]ll the sounds discussed in this book are demonstrated”, according to the book description. I highly recommend this book to anyone with an interest in Spanish language and linguistics and/or phonetics and phonology. (And if you don’t trust me, trust Ralph Penny, who writes: ‘This is a masterful and wide-ranging book. Every student and scholar of Spanish phonetics and phonology should read it and keep it by them.’ Now that’s high praise.)

The second is the excellent book by John M. Lipski that is kind of in between the other two in several respects. It was published in 1994, about half-way between the others. Like Canfield’s book, the bulk of Lipski’s book is also organized country-by-country (though Spanish in the U.S. is not discussed, at least not separately). (See Note 1 below.) The rest of the contentful part of the book consists of a 136pp. introductory part on “the evolution of Latin American Spanish” and a 65pp. bibliography (compare Canfield’s 9pp. and Hualde’s 10pp. bibliographies); Lipski is also more comprehensive in that he discusses syntactic, morphological, and lexical characteristics of different varieties in addition to phonological/phonetic ones. Lipski also provides a useful historical overview for each country.

Why am I bothering to give you all this bibliophilial detail? Well, as Bill Poser and Geoff Nunberg (and everyone else at Language Log) will readily appreciate, books are cool. But it’s also by way of saying: these are just a few of the sources that I have consulted to back up what I said in my Language Log post and what I’m about to say below.


First, more details about Chilean Spanish. On p. viii of the Preface, Canfield writes:

In conversation with a radio announcer from Valparaíso, Chile, I noted that he consciously pronounced /č/ as [š]. When questioned, he stated that he considered it “más suave [= “softer”].” What he thought of as meticulous articulation actually involved the elimination of an occlusive element, a change that occurred in French during the thirteenth century and that is now taking place in cities of northern Chile, in Panama, in Cuba, and elsewhere.

In other words, the relevant change has happened in at least one other Romance language, and it is happening in several different varieties of Spanish. I’m not sure what Canfield means by “consciously pronounced” and “meticulous articulation”, though. Did the radio announcer really try to pronounce a fricative instead of an affricate? Or was he simply aware — especially when being questioned about it specifically! — that these sounds are different in a specific way and that the fricative he uses is non-standard? My guess is that the radio announcer’s “softer” answer is his aesthetic ex post facto reason for why he “chooses” not to use what should otherwise be the preferred, standard affricate pronunciation.

In the chapter on Chile (pp. 31-33), Canfield notes that one “rather general” fact about Chilean speech is that there is an “alveolar rather than palatal articulation of the /č/” — that is, that it sounds more like [ts]. One of Canfield’s maps (the one on the left in this unauthorized reproduction) indicate that this alveolar pronunciation extends from near Antofagasta in the “Big North” through the Center and South portions of the country and into parts of North Patagonia (see this map for the identification of these regions). Valparaíso, where the radio announcer of the Preface is from, is right in the middle of this large swath of the country. Canfield explains further:

There is an interesting trend in some northern towns that may be attitudinal, but since it is also becoming very popular in Panama and to an extent in Cuba and Puerto Rico, it may indicate further development similar to the one that occurred in French in the thirteenth century: the loss of the occlusive element in /č/, which is then heard as [š]. As has been noted (in the Preface), speakers may consider the simple sibilant “más suave.”

Canfield’s other map (the one on the right) shows the handful of small areas, fairly spread apart in the North, where the fricative [š] is found — and Valparaíso is in the southernmost of these areas. (This explains most of this comment, as I point out just after that.) All of this indicates that the phenomenon is more socially than geographically stratified, as also noted by two commenters on my original post (the second of which self-identifies as a “Native Chilean”) and as this Slate Explainer article from Dec. 12 notes (thanks to Ben Zimmer for the link):

The confusion starts with the ch sound, which can serve as a marker of social class in Chilean Spanish. In educated speech, the Spanish ch is similar to the English pronunciation, as in the word chess. But popular dialect turns the ch into something more like sh. A high-class Chilean would probably pronounce the country’s name as “chee-lay,” while someone with less status might say “shee-lay.” Likewise, the same two people might describe the ex-dictator as “pee-no-chay” and “pee-no-shay.” (Pinochet himself was known for speaking in a rough, working-class style. Listen to him pronounce Chile with an sh, about 24 seconds into this video.)

This brings us back to the NPR story that Geoff Pullum originally told me about around the water cooler at Language Log Plaza. Steve Inskeep’s question to Nathan Crooks was specifically about how Pinochet himself pronounced his own name. (Pace this commenter, who claims to know what “Inskeep’s intended question” was; see Note 2 below.) Like Canfield’s radio announcer, Pinochet is from Valparaíso (according to his Wikipedia entry), and the paragraph quoted just above implies that he’d use the fricative [š]. But the same Explainer article continues:

How did Pinochet himself say it? Three different sources told the Explainer they knew or remembered how the general or his family pronounced the name. And they gave three conflicting answers. You can hear Pinochet utter his own name two seconds into this video clip from 1980–it sounds a lot like “pee-no-chay.” If you’ve come across another audio or video clip in which Pinochet or a member of his family pronounces the name, please send it to the Explainer.

To me, the audio quality of the linked video clip is too poor — and Pinochet says his name too fast — to tell definitively whether he says [č] or [š]. (Listen for yourself.) There even seems to be some paper-shuffling sound, much closer to the microphone, right around the time he says “Pinochet”. (On the other hand, it seems fairly clear that he didn’t pronounce the final “t”; if he had, it would be distinguishable in this context, right before a vowel-initial word — “Ugarte”, Pinochet’s second surname.)

The audio quality of the previously linked video clip is much better. (This is the clip in which Pinochet utters “Chile” with what seems like an [š]; you can hear just that part of the clip here.) This clip is an excerpt from an interview, and the topic of the excerpt is about alleged human rights violations in Pinochet’s detention centers. The word for “rights” in Spanish is “derechos”, and Pinochet says it twice in this clip: the first time with a clear [č], and the second time with a clear [š]. (If you’re interested you can see waveforms and spectrograms for these two sound files here and here.)

Anyway, the Explainer article agrees with me: there’s no single way any given Chilean is expected to pronounce “Pinochet” — but that’s only after having tried to select one pronunciation over the others not just once but twice in the past (thanks also to this commenter for pointing me to the first of these):

Slate has twice tried to get to the bottom of this question: Eight years ago we went with “pee-no-CHAY” but then reversed course last year with “pin-oh-CHET.” So, which is it?

All of the above. There’s no single correct pronunciation for the name in Chile. The first two syllables don’t change too much, and should be something between “pin-oh” and “pee-no.” But the last syllable is up for grabs: Some Chileans go with “shay,” others “chay,” and still others “chet.”

And, in the aggregate, the folks who commented on my Language Log post agree. (For the sake of completeness, however, I would follow the lead of Nathan Crooks, and add “shet” to the Explainer’s list of three pronunciations just above.)

I also have to add to the Explainer’s story about the final “t” issue:

It gets more complicated with the final t. As a general rule, the whole syllable–“chet”–should be spoken aloud. But in casual conversation, Chileans tend to drop the final sound. Someone who pronounced Pinochet as “pee-no-chet” would be correct, but he’d also be speaking in a formal (and perhaps a bit uppity) tone. On the other hand, some Chileans are inclined to use the French pronunciation of Pinochet, since the name is of French Basque origin. In that case, they’d drop the t and go back to “pee-no-shay” or “pee-no-chay.”

I don’t know about the “French Basque” business; Pinochet’s Wikipedia entry says his father was a “descendant of Breton immigrants who arrived in Chile during the 18th century”, and Brittany’s quite a way from the Basque country. (Then again, my parents are both from Bolivia but my surname’s Croatian, so I suppose it could happen.) But anyway, as I explained in my Language Log post, not pronouncing a word-final “t” is expected of most Spanish speakers, not just Chileans; there’s no need to invoke the French pronunciation bit.

One final note on this: it may be tough for a typical English speaker to tell whether or not there’s a final [t] in a given pronunciation of “Pinochet” by a Spanish speaker, for reasons I mentioned in passing in my Language Log post. The monophthongal [e] of Spanish sounds more like the [ɛ] of English bed than the diphthong [eɪ] of English bay, and [ɛ] can’t end a word in English (this comment notwithstanding). Unless the cues you’re listening for are the release of the [t] — which you typically wouldn’t get unless there’s a following vowel — you may get fooled by the vowel quality: if the vowel sounds like [ɛ], and you know (subconsciously) that words don’t end in [ɛ], then you may (again, subconsciously) fill in a consonant after that vowel.


And now to correct myself, and to add a little bit more, about [š] in other varieties of Spanish.

First, orthographic (non-final) “y” and “ll” pronounced as [š] is indeed found in the speech of some Uruguayans, but I was wrong to restrict this to Montevideo, and as at least three commenters have so far pointed out, I was wrong to contrast this with Argentinian speech. On Uruguayan Spanish, Canfield (p. 88) writes:

As in a good part of eastern Argentina and in sections of the Northwest, both /lˬ/ and /y/ of the traditional sound system have become [ž], with a tendency toward unvoicing [that is, toward [š]] in recent times, especially among women.

Note that the ‘ˬ’ diacritic is supposed to be under the ‘l’ there; this is Canfield’s voiced palatal lateral symbol (= IPA [ʎ]). Elaborating on Argentinian Spanish, Canfield (p. 24) writes:

In the porteño area and south through Patagonia and east of a line running roughly from Córdoba to Bariloche on the border of Chile, there is leveling [of both /ʎ/ and /y/] to one phoneme whose phonetic form is [ž] and occasionally [š], the latter manifestation becoming more common recently, especially in the speech of women.

(According to Wikipedia, “Porteño is the Spanish demonym for those born in the Argentine city of Buenos Aires“.) One of Canfield’s maps of Argentina — the one on the right — shows the extent of this phenomenon; compare also his broader map of the Americas.

The variety of Spanish that Canfield delimits here (more or less) is known as Rioplatense, which also includes most of Uruguay. (Thanks to Miskwito for the Wikipedia link, where it is also clarified that “either voiceless [ʃ] (this phenomenon is called sheísmo) or voiced [ʒ] (called zheísmo)” are found (in place of /ʎ/ and /y/) in this variety.

Canfield identifies three countries other than Chile with speakers who realize /č/ as [š] (p. 12): “in the younger generation of Panama’s urban centers and sporadically in Cuba, Puerto Rico, and the cities of norther Chile it [that is, /č/] is [š]”.

  • Regarding Panama (p. 67): “Among the youth of Panama, especially in the cities, /č/ is rendered as [š]”.
  • Regarding Cuba (p. 42): “Many observers have noted that the /č/ is weakening to [š] among Cubans, and here again it seems to be more common among women than men.”
  • Regarding Puerto Rico (p. 76): “In the Southwest it [that is, /č/] is [č], and one notes today, as in Cuba, a tendency toward [š] at times.”

Using Lipski’s index I found that he specifically mentions the pronunciation of /č/ as [š] in seven out of his nineteen country-by-country descriptions. Note in particular Lipski’s detailed descriptions of the situations in Panama City (5) and Puerto Rico (7) below, which confirm what I said in Note 1 of my Language Log post … except I said it about Chile. (These particular descriptions also add support to observations made by Penny Eckert and others that younger generations — in particular, young women — “are the movers and shakers in linguistic change”.)

  1. Regarding Argentina generally (p. 169): “The affricate /č/ almost never loses its occlusive elements; if this were to occur, it could potentially create a large number of homonyms, given the prevailing pronunciation of /y/ as [š].”
       But, more specifically with respect to the “Northeast/Guaraní-influenced zone” of the country (p. 171): “In Misiones, some older speakers give a fricative pronunciation to /č/, but in this dialect neither /y/ nor /ʎ/ receives a pronunciation which might result in merger with /č/”.
  2. Regarding Chile (p. 200): “Chilean /č/ is routinely cited as a distinguishing feature of this dialect, in view of its frequent prepalatal articulation, approximating [ts]. This was first described by Lenz (1940: 150). Oroz (1966: 113) disagrees, noting instead a frequent fricative /č/, predominantly in the northern regions. Bernales (1986) confirms the fricative pronunciation of /č/ in southern Chile, while also observing a more forwardly-articulated affricate similar to the one described by Lenz. Wigdorsky (1983) and Diaz Campos (1986) also note the fricative pronunciation.
  3. Regarding Cuba (p. 231): “The affricate /č/ only rarely deaffricates; Canfield (1981: 42) notes incipient deaffrication for Cuba, but it is much less frequent than in Puerto Rico, Panama or western Andalusia.”
  4. Regarding the “Cañar and Azuay (including Cuenca)” areas of Ecuador (p. 249): “The affricate /č/ is sometimes realized as a fricative (Candau 1987).”
  5. Regarding Panama (p. 299): “In Panama City, the affricate /č/ frequently receives a fricative pronunciation (Cedergren 1973). Reduction is more common in word-internal intervocalic position than word-initially. Women apply deaffrication more frequently than men, and the change is of recent origin, spreading among Panama City’s youngest residents. The fricative varaint is used most frequently among middle-class speakers, with frequency of deaffrication dropping off among upper-class speakers and among the lowest classes.”
  6. Regarding the “Andean highlands” of Peru (p. 319): “The affricate /č/ often emerges as a fricative (Escobar 1978: 46).”
       But, in “Lima/central coast” (p. 321): “The affricate /č/ does not normally lose its occlusive element (Escobar 1978: 46).”
  7. Regarding Puerto Rico (p. 332): “The affricate /č/ in Puerto Rican Spanish (especially in the eastern part of the island) was described by Navarro Tomás (1948) as adherente, meaning that the occlusive onset predominates over the fricative continuation. Quilis and Vaquero (1973) and Vaquero (1978) discovered fewer instances of this type of pronunciation, finding an increasing tendency for fricative realization of intervocalic /č/. López Morales (1983a: 147-156) determined that the fricative pronunciation is relatively recent in San Juan, is preferred among women, and is being reversed in the youngest generations. The fricative pronunciation is more frequent in the lower social classes, but only in the urban environment.

Additionally, in the Dominican Republic (p. 238) “the occlusive element [of /č/] generally remains”, and in Venezuela (p. 350) “/č/ rarely loses its occlusive element” (emphasis added in both cases).

Finally, Lipski comments on the [ž] and [š] realizations of /y/ (and /ʎ/) in Argentina and Uruguay:

  • Regarding the “Buenos Aires/southern littoral” area of Argentina (p. 170): “The phoneme /ʎ/ does not exist, and /y/ receives a groove fricative pronunciation known as žeísmo or rehilamiento. Although the original sound was voiced [ž], most younger residents of Buenos Aires now pronounce a voiceless [š], and the devoicing is spreading throughout Argentina.”
  • Regarding Uruguay (p. 340): “The phonemes /y/ and /ʎ/ have merged, giving a groove fricative pronunciation [ž]. Devoicing to [š] is not as all-encompassing in Montevideo as in the Argentine capital [Buenos Aires], but is rapidly gaining ground.”

Rather than quote similar passages from Hualde’s book, I instead have some brief sound clips from the accompanying CD. According to the read-me file included on the CD: “it should be noted that the Buenos Aires speaker [a relatively young-sounding female] consistently employs a voiceless fricative [that is, [š]] in words with orthographic y, ll, instead of the more conservative voiced variant [that is, [ž]] that appears in the Argentinian transcriptions in the book.”

ellos ‘they’ brilló ‘shone’ toalla ‘towel’ playa ‘beach’
yeso ‘plaster’ enyesa ‘he/she plasters’ enyesar ‘to plaster’ el yeso ‘the plaster’
deshielo ‘thaw’ desierto ‘desert’ hierbas ‘grasses’ contrast

The first two examples are selected from a text reading, so they sound cut off; the remaining items were elicited more or less individually. The first row shows a few different words with orthographic “ll” or “y” between vowels that is pronounced as [š]. The second row shows a word that begins with orthographic “y” that is pronounced as [š], followed by variants of the same root morpheme in different contexts. (These happen to be contexts — after a nasal or lateral — where Argentinian speakers who otherwise use voiced [ž] between vowels are reported to produce an affricate [ʤ] (a voiced postalveolar affricate, as in jump or germ in English). The last row contains a few words with a [y] sound (represented orthographically with hi- or i-), demonstrating that Argentinian speakers distinguish [y] from [š]/[ž] — in other words, it’s not that they “can’t pronounce a [y]-sound” or anything like that. The very last cell of the table is a longer sound file specifically showing this contrast; it’s a reading of the examples in Table 9.9 of Hualde’s book (p. 169):

yeso ‘plaster’ hielo ‘ice’
llena ‘full, fem. hiena ‘hyena’
tramoya ‘artifice’, cebolla ‘onion’ paranoia ‘paranoia’
yerbamate leaves’ hierba ‘grass’

That’s about all I’m willing/able to muster on this topic, at least for now. (I have done my best to answer comments on other aspects of the Language Log post in the <a href=”comments area of that post. Feel free to add more comments there or, preferably, here.)


Notes

  1. Concerning “country-by-country classifications”, Lipski (pp. 4-5) writes:

    No serious observer of Latin America would propose that contemporary national boundaries should form the primary variable in determining dialect zones, but there is some value attached to organizing a purely descriptive presentation as a catalogue of national traits […]. The larger [primarily Spanish-speaking] nations in Latin America (e.g. Mexico, Colombia, Argentina and Chile) fully circumscribe entire dialect zones, and even small nations like Ecuador and Costa Rica envelop complete dialect capsules within their borders. Most dialect zones straddle national boundaries, and the only variables which show a close correlation with national boundaries are vocabulary items intimately related to the idiosyncracies of national culture, such as colloquial terms for national political parties or residents of a particular region. Despite the unlikelihood that models based solely on national identity will yield any theoretical insights, most descriptive studies have focused on single nations […], or on cities or regions within a single country.

  2. The main point of the relevant comment is that “Spanish usage is not the determinant of [how to pronounce a name in English]” — something with which I heartily agree, as I’ve written about before here, here, and here. (Also of relevance are the comments here, especially the links provided in Bob Kennedy‘s comment.) Note that this commenter also does not provide an answer to what he claims to be “Inskeep’s intended question”, presumably because all the options are equally permissible (though perhaps not equally frequent/familiar) sequences of close-enough-to-English sounds.

8 thoughts on “More on Pinochet

  1. Matthew Stuckwisch

    The main thing to remember when looking at words that aren’t Spanish is that some people will pronounce them as the origin (especially with names) and others will pronounce with a closer-to-Spanish realisation. For example, I had for a while been mentally pronouncing the name of a character in a drama for one of my grad classes [ˈχi.na] (‹Gina›) as per standard pronounciation rules. However, when we saw a production of it (it’s a recent work, so the author was probably involved), her name was very obviously non-Spanish, with something closer to [ˈʒ.ina] (with maybe a slight occlusive [ˈtʒi.na], though not [ˈʧi.na] or [ˈʤi.na]). It’s not unlike English, where (in SAE, at least) some people will say liason with a distinctly French effect replete with nasalisation [li.eˈsõn̚], but others will simply say [lijˈejᵊz.zən]. The same can be said for a lot of other imported names, who often retain sounds that aren’t even in the local Spanish dialects (lots of names with ‹j› come to mind).

    In other words, regardless of how ‹ch› or ‹⁓t› is said, because it’s imported, it could change. In Spain, at least, I recall ‹⁓t› in names being sometimes [⁓é], and sometimes [⁓éθ], which is a sort of ‹⁓t› < ‹⁓d› morph, since ‹d› and often does ends words, even more so in Castilian Spanish with the imperative y’all form of verbs, but of course others did [⁓ét] sometimes with release, and sometimes not. Eventually I would guess a name like Pinochet would get regularised to Pinoché (much like ‹álbum› is fast becoming ‹albun›, because ‹⁓m› isn’t particularly common in Spanish and said like ‹⁓n› anyways), but I won’t predict that either :)

  2. language hat

    You might want to correct the explanatory bracket here:

    the more conservative voiced variant [that is, [č]]

    I’m afraid you’ve got č’s on the brain! Great post, though.

  3. language hat

    Oh, and also:

    my parents are both from Bolivia but my surname’s Croatian

    I’ve always pronounced your name “BACK-o-vich” in my mind in Anglicized Croatian fashion, but now that I know your folks are from Bolivia I’m all confused. How do Bolivians say it, and how do you?

  4. Eric Bakovic

    Thanks for pointing out the error, hat — there were in fact a couple more like that, which I’m afraid were copy-paste errors performed on html codes for unicode characters. (So, there may be some more; I’ll see if I can catch them before others do.)

    The pronunciation of my own name is an interesting question (to me, anyway). I have to run right now but I’ll post another comment about it later.

  5. Eric Bakovic

    For as long as I can remember, my father (native Spanish speaker, speaks English well but with a discernible accent) has said [ˈbakoβiʧ], but to clarify the spelling he’d say [ˈbakovik]. I think his side of the family does the same. (FWIW, the members of his generation are all first-generation Bolivians whose parents emigrated from Croatia.) The only other variants I’ve heard Bolivians use (e.g., members of my mother’s side of the family) is [bakoˈβiʧ] or [bakoˈβik] — that is, with final instead of initial stress.

    I use the same pronunciations as my father does when I’m speaking Spanish. When I say it in English, I say [ˈbækəvɪʧ] (or [ˈbækəvɪk], if I’m clarifying the spelling). I made a conscious decision to pronounce a final [ʧ] — and to write the acute accent over the ‘c’ (Baković) — when I first visited Croatia in the early 80s. Up until that point I said [ˈbɑ:kəvɪk], which is what my brother still says. I switched from [ɑ:] to [æ] in the first syllable in the early 90s because that’s how new people I was meeting in grad school were saying it, and I just liked it. That said, I don’t care much about these details and I don’t insist on one or another pronunciation (or about the acute accent over the ‘c’).

    There you have it — more than you ever wanted to know about this particular topic. But hat asked, so I answered.

    Now check this: this announcement just happened to come out over LINGUIST List, about an interview with Bill Labov in the Journal of English Linguistics by Matthew J. Gordon. Here’s how the interview begins:

    MG: I’d like to begin by asking about a familiar linguistic variable.
          Do you pronounce your last name [ləbov] or [ləbav]?

    WL: [ləbov]

  6. Julian Fitzgerald

    Regarding your fairly exhaustive Pinochet pronunciation analysis, I might have something to add, just might …

    I used to live in Peru and brought my daughter up speaking Peruvian Spanish with her mum initially. When in England we had a lot of contact with Chilean expatriates and chilenismos. I also studied Sanskrit a little at university – enough to have a good grounding in the principles of phonetics. French is my best foreign language, so I have some way of triangulating this particular surname. In passing, it’s not Basque, irrespective of whether it had a Basque origin.

    As far as I can tell, hypercorrection – the mental map of correct or received pronunciation, coupled with the supposedly invariable pronunciations of each letter in the Spanish alphabet, are what would account for the way Chilean people would think they pronounce Pinochet. How Spanish speakers actually pronounce many letters depending largely on whether they are intervocalic or at the start or end of things, as you say. Spanish is extraordinarily fluid and reliant on phonetic context in a similar way to … Sanskrit, which is why hypercorrection is such a significant phenomenon in Spanish, often odd to the foreign ear, because native speakers will normally swear blind that they always pronounce it in the way they do when spelling it out in mental syllable phonemes. But Chileans are not nearly as rule-bound as some other Spanish-speaking nations (ie Peruvians, who are), and are in fact impossibly inventive.

    People would pronounce Pinochet with the /ch/ sound to add emphasis when shouting in a crowd – that’s why your example of crowd-chanting almost gets to a /d/ or /dj/ sound. Given that a good many Pinochet supporters may be rightwing and snobby, this might add to the pronunciation of the /ch/ sound as opposed to the more normal /sh/ sound in the demotic. The softer /shay/chay/ is actually a Peruvian (possibly also for Chilean and others) slang term for Argentinians – they call them that because they like to think that all Argentinians make that sound all the time, drawing out certain vowels, due to their Italian roots. It makes metropolitan Peruvians from Lima laugh, as they contrast this with Viceregal Spanish from Lima, which they see as staccato and syllable-perfect castellano – where /sh/ doesn’t even exist. Chileans may have ambivalent reasons for using the /che/ and /shay/ sounds due to their unutterable fondness for Argentinians and close proximity. Again that could account for the short assertive /ch/ sound in pro-Pinochet patriots. It’s more a political statement than anything to do with mapping language variants on maps, particularly in the age of mass media – these are markers of political and social identity.

    In normal speech, you’d say shay in Pinochet, but you might say it more /ch/ simply because it is more of a plosive, if you didn’t like him so much. There are masses of Yugoslav and other surnames in Chile, they would be quite accustomed to varied pronunciations in surnames. You’d never say the final t in Pinochet unless it was for a similar reason of abusive over-pronunciation (or perhaps weird hypercorrection at the limit). Oh, and you defintely wouldn’t say it in England or Europe either. I find it difficult to believe that Americans would pronounce it the way they pronounce it for any reason except that they are determined to inflict their version of English – their linguistic imperialism – even on foreign names – which is not actually a “true-English” habit at all … in fact more like a Spanish habit, or that of any other ex-colony which wants to “maintain the language” (in their dreams). Of course that hardly describes Chileans, whose first President was I believe Irish and who had little to do with being a colony of anybody … ever.

    So that is just my two-pence worth.

    Julian Fitzgerald
    Leeds, UK

  7. Laura Martin

    Hi. I have been out of the country and am just now catching up with the Pinochet discussion via Language Log. If you are not totally sick of sh vs. ch in Spanish dialects by now, you might take a look at the relevant sections of my site on Guatemalan Spanish at http://academic.csuohio.edu/guatespn. I have posted a number of articles, examples, and other items about the origins, distribution, and general characteristics of the occurence of sh pronunciations in the Spanish of Guatemala.
    Laura Martin
    Cleveland, Ohio

  8. language hat

    I find it difficult to believe that Americans would pronounce it the way they pronounce it for any reason except that they are determined to inflict their version of English – their linguistic imperialism – even on foreign names – which is not actually a “true-English” habit at all … in fact more like a Spanish habit, or that of any other ex-colony which wants to “maintain the language” (in their dreams).

    What a stupid and insulting remark. In the first place, most Americans pronounce it “PEE-no-shay,” which I gather you think is the proper way. In the second place, Americans are far more likely to honor native pronunciations than Brits, who invented the whole “say Calais as Callis” thing (not to mention the charming attitude that “wogs begin at Calais”). Keep your vulgar nationalism to yourself.

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *